• HOME
  • NEWS
  • EXPLORE
    • CAREER
      • Companies
      • Jobs
    • EVENTS
    • iGEM
      • News
      • Team
    • PHOTOS
    • VIDEO
    • WIKI
  • BLOG
  • COMMUNITY
    • FACEBOOK
    • INSTAGRAM
    • TWITTER
Sunday, June 22, 2025
BIOENGINEER.ORG
No Result
View All Result
  • Login
  • HOME
  • NEWS
  • EXPLORE
    • CAREER
      • Companies
      • Jobs
        • Lecturer
        • PhD Studentship
        • Postdoc
        • Research Assistant
    • EVENTS
    • iGEM
      • News
      • Team
    • PHOTOS
    • VIDEO
    • WIKI
  • BLOG
  • COMMUNITY
    • FACEBOOK
    • INSTAGRAM
    • TWITTER
  • HOME
  • NEWS
  • EXPLORE
    • CAREER
      • Companies
      • Jobs
        • Lecturer
        • PhD Studentship
        • Postdoc
        • Research Assistant
    • EVENTS
    • iGEM
      • News
      • Team
    • PHOTOS
    • VIDEO
    • WIKI
  • BLOG
  • COMMUNITY
    • FACEBOOK
    • INSTAGRAM
    • TWITTER
No Result
View All Result
Bioengineer.org
No Result
View All Result
Home NEWS Science News Neuroscience

Worms’ mental GPS helps them find food

Bioengineer by Bioengineer
December 11, 2014
in Neuroscience
Reading Time: 3 mins read
1
ADVERTISEMENT
Share on FacebookShare on TwitterShare on LinkedinShare on RedditShare on Telegram

You’ve misplaced your cell phone. You start by scanning where you remember leaving it: on your bureau. You check and double-check the bureau before expanding your search around and below the bureau. Eventually, you switch from this local area to a more global one, widening your search to the rest of your room and beyond.

Worms mental GPS helps them find food

Sreekanth Chalasani and Tatyana Sharpee Photo Credits: The Salk Institute for Biological Studies

When it comes to animals and food, a similar strategy is used to search for food (“foraging”). Now, Salk scientists have developed a mathematical theory–based on roundworm foraging–that predicts how animals decide to switch from localized to very broad searching. This new theory could begin to explain animal behavior in a more unified way, laying the groundwork for general rules of behavior that could help us understand complex or erratic attention-related behaviors, such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and even let us predict how extraterrestrials might behave.

“How do you decide which route to take home or which problem to work on? This theory is exploring what ultimately makes us human–how we make decisions based on partial information affects all aspects of our lives,” says Tatyana Sharpee, associate professor of Salk’s Computational Neurobiology Laboratory and senior author of the paper, which was published in eLife on December 9, 2014.

Worms and other animals often follow a chemical trail (a scent, for example) to find their food. But when no chemicals are present they switch to an “infotaxis” search where information gathering happens in more discrete local and global stages.

“What is surprising is that these simple organisms with a very small nervous system perform–or approximate–fairly complex, multistep, long-term planning strategies,” says Sharpee.

This type of search is like looking for a friend at a crowded beach, says Sreekanth Chalasani, assistant professor of Molecular Neurobiology Laboratory, holder of Salk’s Helen McLoraine Developmental Chair in Neurobiology and co-senior author of the work. If you think the friend is under a blue umbrella, you won’t necessarily charge to the first blue umbrella you see. You’ll probably first conduct a more global survey of the scene, glancing at all the umbrellas and ruling out areas where blue does not appear. If you still can’t find your friend, you would expand your search to include other locations in addition to under blue umbrellas. Even though you waste some time, you still obtain a lot of information about where this person is likely to be by focusing on things not directly related to your target.

“Seeking information about a target is often better than seeking the target itself,” says Chalasani. “And what this paper confirms in a theoretical model is that you don’t need lots of neurons to perform these searches that include switching from a local to a global search–you can approximate it by using just three neurons, as in the roundworm C. elegans.”

Successfully seeking food is critical to an animal’s survival. Previously, C. elegans worms have been shown to conduct an intense search of an area where they believe food to be located. After 15 minutes or so, they turn less and explore a more extended area. Since the worms didn’t have a chemical gradient to follow, the team wanted to explore the underlying strategy of the worms’ behavior. Using data from Chalasani’s experimental studies, Sharpee’s lab developed virtual simulations of the worms to model their behavior.

Though this information-maximizing theory has been tested in a few types of behavior, Sharpee and others believe it could provide a basis for a larger, unified framework for understanding different types of behavior across species, cells, neurons and even larger scale phenomenon, such as resource allocation.

Next, the researchers plan to see whether the roundworm can–and to what extent–maintain a mental map of its food search and explore the energy costs and benefits of information searching.

Authors of the paper include Adam Calhoun of the University of California, San Diego, in addition to Sharpee and Chalasani.

The work was funded by a National Science Foundation graduate research fellowship, and the UCSD Institute for Neural Computational graduate fellowship to Adam Calhoun, the Rita Allen Foundation to Sreekanth Chalasani and a National Science Foundation CAREER award the National Institutes of Health, McKnight Endowment Fund for Neuroscience and The Ray Thomas Edwards Foundation to Tatyana Sharpee.

Story Source:

The above story is based on materials provided by Salk Institute for Biological Studies.

Share12Tweet8Share2ShareShareShare2

Related Posts

blank

Redox biomarker could predict progression of epilepsy

October 5, 2016
blank

Neural membrane’s structural instability may trigger multiple sclerosis

October 5, 2016

Scientists find new path in brain to ease depression

October 5, 2016

Key players responsible for learning and memory formation uncovered

October 3, 2016
Please login to join discussion

POPULAR NEWS

  • Green brake lights in the front could reduce accidents

    Study from TU Graz Reveals Front Brake Lights Could Drastically Diminish Road Accident Rates

    161 shares
    Share 64 Tweet 40
  • New Study Uncovers Unexpected Side Effects of High-Dose Radiation Therapy

    76 shares
    Share 30 Tweet 19
  • Pancreatic Cancer Vaccines Eradicate Disease in Preclinical Studies

    71 shares
    Share 28 Tweet 18
  • How Scientists Unraveled the Mystery Behind the Gigantic Size of Extinct Ground Sloths—and What Led to Their Demise

    65 shares
    Share 26 Tweet 16

About

We bring you the latest biotechnology news from best research centers and universities around the world. Check our website.

Follow us

Recent News

New Translational Read-Through Drugs for Fanconi Anemia

Urban Trees Viewed More Negatively Post-COVID Lockdowns

Truncated LKB1 Mimics Smac to Boost Fas Apoptosis

  • Contact Us

Bioengineer.org © Copyright 2023 All Rights Reserved.

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In
No Result
View All Result
  • Homepages
    • Home Page 1
    • Home Page 2
  • News
  • National
  • Business
  • Health
  • Lifestyle
  • Science

Bioengineer.org © Copyright 2023 All Rights Reserved.