• HOME
  • NEWS
  • EXPLORE
    • CAREER
      • Companies
      • Jobs
    • EVENTS
    • iGEM
      • News
      • Team
    • PHOTOS
    • VIDEO
    • WIKI
  • BLOG
  • COMMUNITY
    • FACEBOOK
    • INSTAGRAM
    • TWITTER
Sunday, May 18, 2025
BIOENGINEER.ORG
No Result
View All Result
  • Login
  • HOME
  • NEWS
  • EXPLORE
    • CAREER
      • Companies
      • Jobs
        • Lecturer
        • PhD Studentship
        • Postdoc
        • Research Assistant
    • EVENTS
    • iGEM
      • News
      • Team
    • PHOTOS
    • VIDEO
    • WIKI
  • BLOG
  • COMMUNITY
    • FACEBOOK
    • INSTAGRAM
    • TWITTER
  • HOME
  • NEWS
  • EXPLORE
    • CAREER
      • Companies
      • Jobs
        • Lecturer
        • PhD Studentship
        • Postdoc
        • Research Assistant
    • EVENTS
    • iGEM
      • News
      • Team
    • PHOTOS
    • VIDEO
    • WIKI
  • BLOG
  • COMMUNITY
    • FACEBOOK
    • INSTAGRAM
    • TWITTER
No Result
View All Result
Bioengineer.org
No Result
View All Result
Home NEWS Science News Health

Pink elephants in the brain?

by
August 12, 2024
in Health
Reading Time: 4 mins read
0
ADVERTISEMENT
Share on FacebookShare on TwitterShare on LinkedinShare on RedditShare on Telegram

Our brain interprets visual information by combining what we see with what we already know. A study published in the journal Neuron by researchers at the Champalimaud Foundation, and supported by the CaixaResearch Health Call of the la Caixa Foundation, reveals a mechanism for learning and storing this existing knowledge about the world. They found that neurons are wired to connect seemingly unrelated concepts. This wiring may be crucial for enhancing the brain’s ability to predict what we see based on past experiences, and bring us a step closer to understanding how this process goes awry in mental health disorders.

Meaningless black and white patches or something else?

Credit: David Marr (Book: Vision by David Marr, MIT Press)

Our brain interprets visual information by combining what we see with what we already know. A study published in the journal Neuron by researchers at the Champalimaud Foundation, and supported by the CaixaResearch Health Call of the la Caixa Foundation, reveals a mechanism for learning and storing this existing knowledge about the world. They found that neurons are wired to connect seemingly unrelated concepts. This wiring may be crucial for enhancing the brain’s ability to predict what we see based on past experiences, and bring us a step closer to understanding how this process goes awry in mental health disorders.

How do we learn to make sense of our environment? Over time, our brain builds a hierarchy of knowledge, with higher-order concepts linked to the lower-order features that comprise them. For instance, we learn that cabinets contain drawers and that Dalmatian dogs have black-and-white patches, and not vice versa. This interconnected framework shapes our expectations and perception of the world, allowing us to identify what we see based on context and experience.

“Take an elephant”, says Leopoldo Petreanu, senior author of the la Caixa-funded study. “Elephants are associated with lower-order attributes such as colour, size, and weight, as well as higher-order contexts like jungles or safaris. Connecting concepts helps us understand the world and interpret ambiguous stimuli. If you’re on a safari, you may be more likely to spot an elephant behind the bushes than you would otherwise. Similarly, knowing it’s an elephant makes you more likely to perceive it as grey even in the dim light of dusk. But where in the fabric of the brain is this prior knowledge stored, and how is it learned?”.

The brain’s visual system consists of a network of areas that work together, with lower areas handling simple details (e.g. small regions of space, colours, edges) and higher areas representing more complex concepts (e.g. larger regions of space, animals, faces). Cells in higher areas send “feedback” connections to lower areas, putting them in a position to learn and embed real-world relationships shaped by experience. For instance, cells encoding an “elephant” might send feedback to cells processing features like “grey”, “big” and “heavy”. The researchers therefore set about investigating how visual experience influences the organisation of these feedback projections, whose functional role remains largely unknown.

“We wanted to understand how these feedback projections store information about the world”, says Rodrigo Dias, one of the study’s first authors. “To do this, we examined the effects of visual experience on feedback projections to a lower visual area called V1 in mice. We raised two groups of mice differently: one in a normal environment with regular light exposure, and the other in darkness. We then observed how the feedback connections, and cells they target in V1, responded to different regions of the visual field”.

In mice raised in darkness, the feedback connections and V1 cells directly below them both represented the same areas of visual space. First author Radhika Rajan picks up the story, “It was amazing to see how well the spatial representations of higher and lower areas matched up in the dark-reared mice. This suggests that the brain has an inherent, genetic blueprint for organising these spatially aligned connections, independent of visual input”. However, in normally-reared mice, these connections were less precisely matched, and more feedback inputs conveyed information from surrounding areas of the visual field.

Rajan continues, “We found that with visual experience, feedback provides more contextual and novel information, enhancing the ability of V1 cells to sample information from a broader area of the visual scene”. This effect depended on the origin within the higher visual area: feedback projections from deeper layers were more likely to convey surround information compared to those from superficial layers.

Moreover, the team discovered that in normally-reared mice, deep-layer feedback inputs to V1 become organised according to the patterns they “prefer” to see, such as vertical or horizontal lines. “For instance”, Dias says, “inputs that prefer vertical lines avoid sending surround information to areas located along the vertical direction. In contrast, we found no such bias in connectivity in dark-reared mice”.

“This suggests that visual experience plays a crucial role in fine-tuning feedback connections and shaping the spatial information transmitted from higher to lower visual areas”, notes Petreanu. “We developed a computational model that shows how experience leads to a selection process, reducing connections between feedback and V1 cells whose representations overlap too much. This minimises redundancy, allowing V1 cells to integrate a more diverse range of feedback”.

Perhaps counterintuitively, the brain might encode learned knowledge by connecting cells that represent unrelated concepts, and that are less likely to be activated together based on real-world patterns. This could be an energy-efficient way to store information, so that when encountering a novel stimulus, like a pink elephant, the brain’s preconfigured wiring maximises activation, enhancing detection and updating predictions about the world.

Identifying this brain interface where prior knowledge combines with new sensory information could be valuable for developing interventions in cases where this integration process malfunctions. As Petreanu concludes, “Such imbalances are thought to occur in conditions like autism and schizophrenia. In autism, individuals may perceive everything as novel because prior information isn’t strong enough to influence perception. Conversely, in schizophrenia, prior information could be overly dominant, leading to perceptions that are internally generated rather than based on actual sensory input. Understanding how sensory information and prior knowledge are integrated can help address these imbalances”.



Journal

Neuron

DOI

10.1016/j.neuron.2024.07.009

Method of Research

Experimental study

Subject of Research

Animals

Article Title

Visual experience reduces the spatial redundancy between cortical feedback inputs and primary visual cortex neurons

Article Publication Date

12-Aug-2024

Share12Tweet8Share2ShareShareShare2

Related Posts

Matrix Metalloproteinase-10 Drives Kidney Fibrosis via β-Catenin

Matrix Metalloproteinase-10 Drives Kidney Fibrosis via β-Catenin

May 17, 2025
blank

Obesity Drugs Aid Weight Loss After Bariatric Surgery

May 17, 2025

METTL13 Controls MYC, Drives Leukemia Cell Survival

May 17, 2025

Low-Dose Radiotherapy Combo Shows Promise in Head and Neck Cancer

May 17, 2025

POPULAR NEWS

  • blank

    Volatile-Rich Cap Found Above Yellowstone Magma

    665 shares
    Share 266 Tweet 166
  • Natural Supplement Shows Potential to Slow Biological Aging and Enhance Muscle Strength

    89 shares
    Share 36 Tweet 22
  • Analysis of Research Grant Terminations at the National Institutes of Health

    78 shares
    Share 31 Tweet 20
  • The Rise of Eukaryotic Cells: An Evolutionary Algorithm Spurs a Major Biological Transition

    68 shares
    Share 27 Tweet 17

About

We bring you the latest biotechnology news from best research centers and universities around the world. Check our website.

Follow us

Recent News

Matrix Metalloproteinase-10 Drives Kidney Fibrosis via β-Catenin

Obesity Drugs Aid Weight Loss After Bariatric Surgery

METTL13 Controls MYC, Drives Leukemia Cell Survival

  • Contact Us

Bioengineer.org © Copyright 2023 All Rights Reserved.

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In
No Result
View All Result
  • Homepages
    • Home Page 1
    • Home Page 2
  • News
  • National
  • Business
  • Health
  • Lifestyle
  • Science

Bioengineer.org © Copyright 2023 All Rights Reserved.