• HOME
  • NEWS
  • EXPLORE
    • CAREER
      • Companies
      • Jobs
    • EVENTS
    • iGEM
      • News
      • Team
    • PHOTOS
    • VIDEO
    • WIKI
  • BLOG
  • COMMUNITY
    • FACEBOOK
    • INSTAGRAM
    • TWITTER
Monday, October 20, 2025
BIOENGINEER.ORG
No Result
View All Result
  • Login
  • HOME
  • NEWS
  • EXPLORE
    • CAREER
      • Companies
      • Jobs
        • Lecturer
        • PhD Studentship
        • Postdoc
        • Research Assistant
    • EVENTS
    • iGEM
      • News
      • Team
    • PHOTOS
    • VIDEO
    • WIKI
  • BLOG
  • COMMUNITY
    • FACEBOOK
    • INSTAGRAM
    • TWITTER
  • HOME
  • NEWS
  • EXPLORE
    • CAREER
      • Companies
      • Jobs
        • Lecturer
        • PhD Studentship
        • Postdoc
        • Research Assistant
    • EVENTS
    • iGEM
      • News
      • Team
    • PHOTOS
    • VIDEO
    • WIKI
  • BLOG
  • COMMUNITY
    • FACEBOOK
    • INSTAGRAM
    • TWITTER
No Result
View All Result
Bioengineer.org
No Result
View All Result
Home NEWS Science News Health

Why do(n’t) people support being nudged towards healthier diets?

Bioengineer by Bioengineer
February 15, 2024
in Health
Reading Time: 6 mins read
0
Researchers conducted an online survey to analyse support for five different nudge scenarios. Then, one aspect of the design was changed, allowing the researchers to pinpoint how these design variations affected public support.
Share on FacebookShare on TwitterShare on LinkedinShare on RedditShare on Telegram

You may not realise it, but “nudge” has been used by businesses, policy-makers and governments for years to prod the public into making different choices. Small changes in our environment can “nudge” us into different behaviours without restricting the options available to us. For example, printing the low-calorie options in bold on a menu, or showing the calorie information, might change what we choose to eat. But does the public support this? And how do subtleties in how ‘nudge’ interventions are designed affect support, if at all? Research led by the Universities of Göttingen and Bonn set out to examine public support for nudge scenarios with different design variations, each aimed at promoting healthy and/or sustainable food choices. The researchers showed there were two promising ways to improve public support for nudging strategies: reducing the effort people must expend to avoid the nudged option they would usually want to follow; and improving the transparency of the nudge. The results were published in BMC Public Health.

Researchers conducted an online survey to analyse support for five different nudge scenarios. Then, one aspect of the design was changed, allowing the researchers to pinpoint how these design variations affected public support.

Credit: Simone Wahnschafft

You may not realise it, but “nudge” has been used by businesses, policy-makers and governments for years to prod the public into making different choices. Small changes in our environment can “nudge” us into different behaviours without restricting the options available to us. For example, printing the low-calorie options in bold on a menu, or showing the calorie information, might change what we choose to eat. But does the public support this? And how do subtleties in how ‘nudge’ interventions are designed affect support, if at all? Research led by the Universities of Göttingen and Bonn set out to examine public support for nudge scenarios with different design variations, each aimed at promoting healthy and/or sustainable food choices. The researchers showed there were two promising ways to improve public support for nudging strategies: reducing the effort people must expend to avoid the nudged option they would usually want to follow; and improving the transparency of the nudge. The results were published in BMC Public Health.

 

People can be nudged to make a certain choice by making it the default option. For instance, rather than offering butter automatically at a restaurant by default, restaurants could make it so that butter is only available upon active request. This type of nudge – known as a “default nudge” – can be effective, but it can be unpopular relative to other nudging strategies. Researchers set out to analyse consumer reaction by conducting an online survey on a sample (N = 451) of German adults, who were presented with five nudge scenarios and prompted to rate their support for each. Participants were also asked in each scenario to indicate what their typical behaviour would be (i.e., would you usually eat butter in a restaurant), the extent to which they perceived the nudge to intrude upon their freedom of choice, and how effective they believed the nudge would be. Participants then answered the same questions for a variation of each nudge scenario in which one aspect of the design was changed, allowing the researchers to pinpoint how these design variations affected public support.

 

The researchers discovered that some designs were more promising than others for improving public support. For instance, reducing the effort needed to opt out of the nudged option – such as by presenting vegetarian dishes on the first pages of a menu followed by meat dishes, rather than providing only a vegetarian menu on the table with a menu with meat options available on request – increased support. Similarly, increasing the transparency of the nudge itself, such as by asking participants whether they preferred a pre-filled ‘climate friendly’ online grocery cart rather than simply offering it by default, increased support. With regard to predicting the level of support, the perception that nudges intruded upon free choice was the most important driver of non-acceptance, whereas the perception of effectiveness was the most salient driver of acceptance.

 

“Understanding public support – and its drivers – is important for designing politically viable, ethical, and effective nudges,” says first author Simone Wahnschafft at Göttingen University’s Sustainable Food Systems research group. “We were surprised to find that the personal circumstances of our participants and whether their own behaviour would be affected by the nudge had little effect on their support. We found that the perception of upholding free choice and of effectiveness were key to public support.” This study opens up avenues for future research into how ‘sweet spots’ can be found for default nudges that are both effective and widely supported.

 

This research was made possible thanks to funding from the German Research Foundation (DFG).

 

Original publication: Lemken and Wahnschafft et al. (2023) ‘Public acceptance of default nudges to promote healthy and sustainable food choices’, BMC Public Health, 10.1186/s12889-023-17127-z

 

Contact:

Simone Wahnschafft

University of Göttingen

Research Training Group in Sustainable Food Systems

Heinrich-Düker-Weg 12, 37073 Göttingen, Germany

Email: [email protected] 

 

Professor Dominic Lemken

University of Bonn

Institute for Food and Resource Economics

Nußallee 21, 53115, Bonn, Germany

Email: [email protected]

You may not realise it, but “nudge” has been used by businesses, policy-makers and governments for years to prod the public into making different choices. Small changes in our environment can “nudge” us into different behaviours without restricting the options available to us. For example, printing the low-calorie options in bold on a menu, or showing the calorie information, might change what we choose to eat. But does the public support this? And how do subtleties in how ‘nudge’ interventions are designed affect support, if at all? Research led by the Universities of Göttingen and Bonn set out to examine public support for nudge scenarios with different design variations, each aimed at promoting healthy and/or sustainable food choices. The researchers showed there were two promising ways to improve public support for nudging strategies: reducing the effort people must expend to avoid the nudged option they would usually want to follow; and improving the transparency of the nudge. The results were published in BMC Public Health.

 

People can be nudged to make a certain choice by making it the default option. For instance, rather than offering butter automatically at a restaurant by default, restaurants could make it so that butter is only available upon active request. This type of nudge – known as a “default nudge” – can be effective, but it can be unpopular relative to other nudging strategies. Researchers set out to analyse consumer reaction by conducting an online survey on a sample (N = 451) of German adults, who were presented with five nudge scenarios and prompted to rate their support for each. Participants were also asked in each scenario to indicate what their typical behaviour would be (i.e., would you usually eat butter in a restaurant), the extent to which they perceived the nudge to intrude upon their freedom of choice, and how effective they believed the nudge would be. Participants then answered the same questions for a variation of each nudge scenario in which one aspect of the design was changed, allowing the researchers to pinpoint how these design variations affected public support.

 

The researchers discovered that some designs were more promising than others for improving public support. For instance, reducing the effort needed to opt out of the nudged option – such as by presenting vegetarian dishes on the first pages of a menu followed by meat dishes, rather than providing only a vegetarian menu on the table with a menu with meat options available on request – increased support. Similarly, increasing the transparency of the nudge itself, such as by asking participants whether they preferred a pre-filled ‘climate friendly’ online grocery cart rather than simply offering it by default, increased support. With regard to predicting the level of support, the perception that nudges intruded upon free choice was the most important driver of non-acceptance, whereas the perception of effectiveness was the most salient driver of acceptance.

 

“Understanding public support – and its drivers – is important for designing politically viable, ethical, and effective nudges,” says first author Simone Wahnschafft at Göttingen University’s Sustainable Food Systems research group. “We were surprised to find that the personal circumstances of our participants and whether their own behaviour would be affected by the nudge had little effect on their support. We found that the perception of upholding free choice and of effectiveness were key to public support.” This study opens up avenues for future research into how ‘sweet spots’ can be found for default nudges that are both effective and widely supported.

 

This research was made possible thanks to funding from the German Research Foundation (DFG).

 

Original publication: Lemken and Wahnschafft et al. (2023) ‘Public acceptance of default nudges to promote healthy and sustainable food choices’, BMC Public Health, 10.1186/s12889-023-17127-z

 

Contact:

Simone Wahnschafft

University of Göttingen

Research Training Group in Sustainable Food Systems

Heinrich-Düker-Weg 12, 37073 Göttingen, Germany

Email: [email protected] 

 

Professor Dominic Lemken

University of Bonn

Institute for Food and Resource Economics

Nußallee 21, 53115, Bonn, Germany

Email: [email protected]

 



Journal

BMC Public Health

DOI

10.1186/s12889-023-17127-z

Method of Research

Survey

Subject of Research

People

Article Title

Public acceptance of default nudges to promote healthy and sustainable food choices’

Article Publication Date

22-Nov-2023

Share12Tweet8Share2ShareShareShare2

Related Posts

Research Confirms: Stimulating the Senses Soothes the Mind

October 20, 2025

Dimethyl Fumarate Boosts Antitumor Immunity in Cervical Cancer

October 20, 2025

Impact of Federal Policies on Hospice Antipsychotic Use

October 20, 2025

Assessing ChatGPT’s Alignment with Geriatric Assessment Experts

October 20, 2025

POPULAR NEWS

  • Sperm MicroRNAs: Crucial Mediators of Paternal Exercise Capacity Transmission

    1268 shares
    Share 506 Tweet 317
  • Stinkbug Leg Organ Hosts Symbiotic Fungi That Protect Eggs from Parasitic Wasps

    301 shares
    Share 120 Tweet 75
  • New Study Suggests ALS and MS May Stem from Common Environmental Factor

    129 shares
    Share 52 Tweet 32
  • ESMO 2025: mRNA COVID Vaccines Enhance Efficacy of Cancer Immunotherapy

    112 shares
    Share 45 Tweet 28

About

We bring you the latest biotechnology news from best research centers and universities around the world. Check our website.

Follow us

Recent News

Linking HPV, EBV, Polyomaviruses to Thyroid Tumors

Research Confirms: Stimulating the Senses Soothes the Mind

Global Increase in Obesity-Linked Cancers Among Both Younger and Older Adults

Subscribe to Blog via Email

Enter your email address to subscribe to this blog and receive notifications of new posts by email.

Join 65 other subscribers
  • Contact Us

Bioengineer.org © Copyright 2023 All Rights Reserved.

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In
No Result
View All Result
  • Homepages
    • Home Page 1
    • Home Page 2
  • News
  • National
  • Business
  • Health
  • Lifestyle
  • Science

Bioengineer.org © Copyright 2023 All Rights Reserved.