Researchers are one step closer to understanding the ‘internal GPS’ of animals and humans, by investigating whether rats can learn spaces just by observation. In a new study published in Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, the researchers show that rats do not need to physically explore an environment to learn about a specific location; simply observing another rat is sufficient.
Credit: Thomas Doublet
Researchers are one step closer to understanding the ‘internal GPS’ of animals and humans, by investigating whether rats can learn spaces just by observation. In a new study published in Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, the researchers show that rats do not need to physically explore an environment to learn about a specific location; simply observing another rat is sufficient.
Learning by observation has been reported in invertebrates (for example in bees), birds, fish, and mammals. Learning new tasks and environments is critical to the survival and well-being of an individual.
“Learning by observation is the most common form of learning from school to daily life,” said author Dr Thomas Doublet, of the Norwegian University of Science and Technology.
Brain maps
Research has shown that animals and humans can navigate distances and spaces thanks to the formation of cognitive maps. Functional cell types have been suggested that underlie cognitive mapping processes in the brain, among them grid cells, border cells, head direction cells, and place cells. A place cell, for example, is a neuron in the hippocampus that becomes active when an animal enters a particular space, which is known as the place field.
The processes that control these cognitive maps and the suspected accompanying cells are still poorly understood, including whether they can be formed in spaces that are only observed or whether direct experience of a space is needed.
Rats are excellent animals when studying these cognitive brain maps as their navigational abilities are well-known. A previous study has shown that their brains create a stable place cell map of an unexplored space only after direct exploration. This raises the question whether rats can learn spaces by observation only, without any exploration beforehand.
To find an answer to this question, Doublet and his colleagues designed an observational spatial task that is nearly identical to that of the previous study.
Learning a reward location
“We wanted to understand whether or not a spatial representation could be acquired remotely,” explained Doublet. “This is important to understand how spatial representations can be generated and stabilized.”
The researchers designed a two-part cage, with an inner cage and an outer cage, where an observer rat (in the inner cage) learned about the location of a food reward only by observing a demonstrator rat (in the outer cage). After the observational training was done, the observer rat could explore the outer cage and find the reward.
The performance of the observer rats was compared to the performance of naive rats, ie rats that had to find the reward without previous observational training.
The observer rats outperformed the naive rats (100% success versus 12% success), meaning that rats can learn about a physical space and can find a reward location with no previous physical exploration; simply observing another rat is sufficient.
Furthermore, the researchers injected CPP (a receptor antagonist which prevents stabilization of a newly formed hippocampal representation of a space but does not destabilize an already formed one), to test whether it would affect the rats’ performance. Injection of CPP did not affect the observational task, meaning that either the observed space was stabilized by observation alone, or that a stable place cell representation is not necessary for spatial task performance.
Our internal GPS
“Our study shows that the cognitive representation of a space formed by observation is stable and can be used by the animals to navigate more efficiently through the observed space,” noted Doublet. “This study is a means to better understand how our brain represents the behavior of conspecifics, but also how our internal GPS works.”
“While these findings may be an important insight to better understand our internal GPS,” he continued, “this study does not explain what exactly this stable representation is or how it is generated. Whether we use the same neurons to represent observed and self-experience remains to be found out.”
Journal
Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience
DOI
10.3389/fnbeh.2022.902675